Science in Christian Perspective
Some Recent Findings in the Neurosciences and their Relevance to
Christianity
C. DANIEL GEISLER
Professor of Neurophysiology Professor of Electrical and Computer
Engineering
University of Wisconsin-Madison
Madison, Wisconsin
From: JASA 28 (December 1976: 156-164
The basic building block of the vertebrate nervous system is the nerve cell. Interactions among nerve cells are accomplished by several means, chief among which are electrical pulses sent along each cell's enclosing membrane, These pulses cause the release of chemicals, called neurotransmitters, which affect neighboring cells. These neurotransmitters can influence conscious experience, as shown by the close connection between antipsychotic drugs and the neurotransmitter dopamine. As knowledge of brain function and the accompanying powers to control people accumulate, many questions are underscored. Who will control the new powers? Can the human brain be considered a computer? The answer to the last question is unknown. The organization of the brain, with its array of 1O° interconnected nerve cells, is far too complex for complete analysis by present methods. Moreover, it appears that a random component exists in the pulse patterns generated by all known nerve cells. Although not conclusive, this randomness suggests that any deterministic model of the brain would, in principle, be inaccurate. Thus, scientists may never be able to describe fully the reasons why a particular brain behaves as it does.
Modern scientific study of the brain is raising issues that are being
taken seriously
by an increasing number of people. With recent successes in measuring
and manipulating
various brain processes and in devising mathematical and computer
models for them,
brain
scientists are currently gaining much deeper insights into the structures and
functions of the brain. A few workers in several different areas of
brain research
have even concluded that enough is known about the functioning of the brain to
show that it is a completely mechanistic machine."1,2 As it is difficult to visualize how such a machine
could have free will and dignity, characteristics which most Christians believe
to be intrinsic attributes of a human personality, these conclusions have been
strenuously resisted. Others, fearful of the growing power of
scientists to manipulate
the human brain, have given warnings about the possible abuse of
these powers.3,4
Thus, modem attempts to unravel the mysteries surrounding the nature
of the human
brain have become of considerable interest to Christianity. It is the purpose
of this paper to describe a few of the newly discovered
characteristics of brain
function and to discuss some of their implications for Christian thought.
As in many fields of science, the study of brain mechanisms has
tended to he concentrated
according to various levels of complexity. Some scientists are
investigating the
behavior of single molecules or groups of molecules, while others investigate
the structure and function of single nerve cells or of particular
neural subsystems.
Still others study the behavior of whole organisms. New findings in
all of these
areas are relevant to our purposes, but attention is focused here on
the recently
discovered properties of single nerve cells, particularly the
mechanisms by which
they communicate rapidly with each other.
BRAIN FUNCTION
The Neuron5
The basic building block of the vertebrate nervous system is the
nerve cell, called
a neuron. Neurons come in many shapes and sizes, but there are certain features
common to all of them. As an example, a schematic drawing of a common
neuron found
in the cat's spinal cord is shown in Fig. 1. This neuron looks somewhat like a
tree, with root-like dendrites, a long slender trunk callel the axon,
and branch-like
axon terminations. There is also a roughly spherical cell body which contains
the cell nucleus and is concerned with maintaining the overall health
of the cell.
Attention should be drawn to the outline of the cell. The lines in
Fig. 1 delineating
it represent a very thin skin-like membrane which completely
surrounds the neuron.
This membrane, which is approximately 10-5 mm (10-8 meters) in
thickness, is itself
an active part of the neuron and separates the inside of the cell,
with its unique
properties, from its surroundings. An electrical voltage of roughly 60 mV (0.06
volts) exists across the membrane.
This may seem like a tiny voltage, but remember that it exists across a very thin membrane. At those submicroscopic dimensions, it produces quite a strong electrical effect, comparable to those in existence in modern electronic devices.
Shown in Fig. 2 is a plot of how changes in this membrane voltage might look as
a function of time. For the first 2 msec (0.002 seconds) of the
graph, the voltage
is observed to vary rather randomly about an average value of zero. At 2 msec,
a slow increase in the voltage begins so that at 3 msec the voltage has risen
to 10 mV. Once the cell voltage has passed that value, a remarkable
event occurs.
A short voltage pulse of almost 100 my is generated. Moreover, once
the membrane
has had a chance to reset itself, it will generate a similar pulse
again and again,
whenever this critical voltage level, called the threshold level, is
crossed (e.g.,
at 7 msee in Fig. 2). Notice that the height and shape of the two pulses shown
in the figure are almost identical. The pulse height is determined by
differences
in ion concentrations within and without the cell. Because these concentrations
are similar for all neurons, all neuronal pulses have about the same size.
The voltage trace shown in Fig. 2 represents the voltage across a
patch of membrane
located near the junction of the cell body and the axon. For the type of neuron
shown, this particular region has the lowest threshold level, and
neuronal pulses
are generated there before anywhere alse. Once generated in the
junction region,
the neuronal pulse has a strong influence on neighboring regions of membrane.
The positive nature of the pulse raises the voltage across the adjacent patch
of membrane, causing that voltage to cross its own threshold level. A neuronal
pulse is then generated by this second patch of membrane and causes, in turn,
the third section of membrane to generate a pulse. The first section
is meanwhile
resetting itself and is not affected by the pulse on the second
section. The process
continues on down the axon, each section generating a pulse which
causes a pulse
in the succeeding section. The process is very much like the burning
of a firecracker
fuse, where the heat of the burning section of the fuse ignites the
next section.
In both eases, a signal is transmitted from one end to the other, a heat pulse
in one ease and a voltage pulse in the other. Moreover, in both cases
the length
of the signal path does not matter. Once started, the pulse propagates at a constant speed to the end of the line. Unlike the firecracker fuse
which burns
but once, the axon resets itself in a msec or so and is ready to
conduct another
pulse. It will conduct many millions of pulses over the course of its lifetime.
The utility of such a mechanism is clear: pulses can be sent over arbitrarily
long distances without any loss of signal. Thus, although only 102 mm
in diameter,
the axon of the neuron depicted in Fig. 1 conducts its pulses from
the cell body
which lies in the spinal cord to a muscle located, let us say, in the foot, a
distance of approximately one meter. There is a price paid for this
"lossless"
transmission of pulses over long distances. Not only does it involve
an expenditure
of energy to keep the axon in readiness to gencrate pulses, but the only type
of signals that can be sent along the axon are pulses. Subthreshold voltages,
such as characterize the first 3 msec of the membrane voltage shown in Fig. 2,
fade away within a few mm.
Before the neuronal pulse reaches the end of the axon, we must pause
and briefly
consider just how the axons terminate. Work with the electron
microscope has revealed
that, even to its very tip, each axon is totally surrounded by the
cell membrane,
but that very close, specialized connections are made with a certain number of
other cells.6 In the brain, these connections are made to other
neurons, but axons
leaving the brain may also make connections with muscle fibers and other types
of cells.
Figure 3 shows schematically a neuron-to-neuron connection, which is
known as a synapse. On the left or delivering side of the synapse in
Fig. 3, the
cell membrane is thickened a hit and there are a number of small
spherical particles
known as vesicles located in the immediate vicinity. Just opposite,
the membrane
on the right side, which may be a patch located on a dendrite or the cell body
or even in rare cases on the axon of the receiving neuron, is also
thickened and
presumably specially adapted for its synaptic role. Although only one synapse
is shown in Fig. 3, a single axon usually makes many synaptic connections along
the course of its termination.
Let us return now to the neuronal pulse as it reaches the end of the
axon terminations.
Just as the purpose of igniting a firecracker fuse is to deliver heat
to the firecracker
itself, so the purpose of the neuronal pulse is to deliver a voltage change to
the membrane of the synaptic region at the end of the axon. That
purpose accomplished,
the neuronal pulse vanishes, without having any direct effect on the receiving
neuron.
The next stage in the process is a chemical one.7 Upon arrival of the voltage
change in the synaptic region, minute packets of a chemical compound
are emitted
from the axon into the gap between the two neurons. Although not
absolutely certain,
there is strong evidence that the total contents of one of the
spherical vesicles
clustered in the synaptic region make up one packet. Upon arrival in the narrow
synaptic cleft, the molecules of the chemical compound are bounced
around by other
molecules and quickly arrive at the membrane of the receiving neuron.
There, the
emitted molecules form linkages with special sites on the receiving
membrane that
are very precisely constructed for the reception of that type of
molecule. After
a brief linkage, the emitted molecules break free and most of them, by various
processes, are absorbed back into the emitting neuron for recycling.
During its brief existence, the linkage between the receptor site and the emitted molecule causes a change to occur in
the properties
of the receiving neuron's membrane. This structural change in turn
causes a small
voltage change to appear in the receiving neuron. If enough emitted molecules
link up with the receiving membrane in a short time, the sum of all
their voltage
changes might be enough to carry the voltage of the trigger zone of
the receiving
cell past the threshold level, and a neuronal pulse would be generated on the
axon of the receiving cell.8 Thus, through the use of a chemical
intermediate,
a voltage change is produced in the receiving neuron by the neuronal pulse on
the emitting axon. As a recognition of its message-carrying nature,
the chemical
used as the intermediate is known as a neurotransmitter.
Not all of the interactions between neurons are carried on by means
of nerve pulses,
the only known method of rapid interoeuron communication over long distances.
Other modes are used when neurons lie near each other. For example, dendrites
of neighboring neurons may form close contacts having all of the
signs of chemical
transmission: vesicles localized in one dendrite and thickened cell membranes
existing on both sides of the synaptic cleft.9 Thus, the
transmission of information
between these dendrites appears to be by means of neurotransmitters.
In this case,
however, the release of the chemical is not necessarily triggered by a neural
impulse, but may be released by the smaller sub-threshold voltages that exist
across the cell membrane in that region. It has been estimated that up to 50%
of the brain may be composed of locally interacting circuits,10 where
transmitter
release is governed by these sub-threshold cell potentials and by neural pulses
conducted on very short axons. There is evidence that some of these
neighborhood
interactions may even be by direct electrical means, without the use
of chemical
transmitters.11
A Neurotransmitter
Some of the most exciting recent discoveries in the neural sciences have been
concerned with oeurotransmitters.7 The use of the plural form of the
word is deliberate,
for it is well established that not all neurons use the same neurotransmitter chemical, Two compounds,
acetylcholine and noradrenaline, have been positively identified as neurotransmitters. That is,
both compounds possess the complete set of specific characteristics
that neurochemists
have established as essential for a neurotransmitter. Nine other
compounds present
in the brain have been identified as possible neurotransmitters, but
as yet they
have not been shown to possess all of the needed properties. Although each of
these compounds merits extensive discussion, we will consider only
dopamine, one
of the nine partially proven neurotransmitters. Effects attributed to
its presence
are very impressive and are closely tied into conscious human experience.
Dopamine seems to serve several purposes in the brain. Its clearest role is in
connection with the proper functioning of the muscular nervous system. Not long
ago, it was discovered that the brains of some patients who had died
of Parkinsonism,
a disease which produces uncontrollable shaking in its victims, had an abnormal
dopamine content. In these brains, a particular region that is normally rich in
dopamine, due to dopamine-containing axons which terminate there, was found to
have virtually no dopamine. In attempts to
The excellent correlation of the data is indeed strong evidence that the powerful therapeutic effect of the antipsychotic drugs is related to the reduction of the flow of dopamine between neurons.
supply the missing dopamine, it was further discovered that injecting a closely
related compound, dopa, into the blood of Parkinson's disease
patients, dramatically
helped to relieve their symptoms. Apparently, the dopa molecules had been able
to cross into the brain, and there they had been changed into the
needed dopamine
by a brain enzyme known to promote this transformation. Thus,
although the precise
roles of dopamine. releasing axons in the neural circuits involving
muscular control
are unknown at present, it seems clear that these roles are of major
importance.
Another role for dopamine is in the process of being established. Since their
initial appearance in the 1950's, there have been a number of drugs developed
for the treatment of schizophrenia. Many of these drugs are quite
different from
each other, but all have antipsyehotie
effects. Research involving these drugs has shown that they also have in
common the ability to block the transmission of dopamine between
neurons. Although
the precise mechanisms of this blockage are still in debate,12,13
there is evidence
which suggests that the drugs inhibit the release of the dopamine from the ends
of the emitting axons. Figure 4, a drawing taken directly from a recent report,13 shows the doses of the different antipsychotic drugs needed to reduce by 50%
the amount of dopamine released by electrical stimulation of excised
brain slices,
compared to the average clinical doses used for controlling schizophrenia. The
correlation between the two measures is really remarkable. Although
the different
clinical doses vary by more than 10,0000-to-i, they can be used to predict with
extreme accuracy the dopamine-inhibiting effect. It can be seen, for example,
that quite high doses of chlorpromazine are prescribed for
controlling schizophrenia,
and that quite high doses of that drug are also needed to inhibit the release
of dopamine from brain slices. Only the few points at the extreme top
and bottom
of the figure deviate significantly from the relationship shown by the straight
line. Although the brains of rats, from which the electrically excited samples
were obtained, are obviously very different from human brains, they have many
biochemical similarities. Thus, the excellent correlation of the data in Fig.
4 is indeed strong evidence that the powerful therapeutic effect of
the antipsyehotie
drugs is related to the reduction of the flow of dopamine between
neurons. However,
even if this particular relationship were to be confirmed, much would
remain mysterious.
For example, it is not even
clear which of the several kinds of dopamine-containing
neurons are involved in the tranquilizing reactions evoked by the antipsyehotie
drugs. More fundamentally, the relationships which exist between
neural activity
and conscious experience of any kind are almost completely unknown.
Neuronal Pulses
For the last two decades, neuronal pulses have been directly
observable by means
of microeleetrodes. These devices, which in principle amount to small
wires sharpened
to a very fine point, have minute tips that can be positioned either
just inside
or just outside a single neuron. With that positioning, each pulse generated by
that neuron causes a very small electrical signal to flow through the
mieroeleetrode.
Electronic amplifiers increase the size of the signals up to the level needed
by the pulse analyzing equipment used, principally the computer. This
combination
of the mieroeleetrode to record neuronal signals and the computer to
analyze them
has been a very productive one. Using them, brain scientists have been able to
investigate pulse patterns generated by neurons in many different
regions of the
brain. As an example of the kind of information being gathered, let us consider
the pattern of pulses which has been recorded from neurons of the
ear. These patterns
are among the simplest in the vertebrate nervous system, and extensive studies
have revealed the main outlines of their behavior.14
There are approximately 50,000 neurons that send information from each ear into
the brain of the eat, a common experimental animal. These neurons have cell
bodies and single dendrites situated in the bony parts of the ear,
and their axons
extend to just inside the brain of the animal. The obvious purpose of
these neurons
is to carry information into the brain concerning the sound signals
striking the
ear. Figure 5 shows an example of how these neurons respond when a pure tone,
say middle C, is sounded in the ear. The top line of the figure shows
the waveform
of the air pressure changes caused by the tone, and the middle line shows the
neuronal pulse pattern that would typically be observed on a single one of the
axons leading into the brains. At first glance, the pattern is not impressive.
There are relatively few pulses and they seem to occur rather
randomly. Sometimes
a long interval separates two adjacent pulses and sometimes they occur in quick
succession. On further investigation, however, it is found that there
is a great
deal of orderliness in the pattern. Either a single pulse or none at
all is generated
during any one cycle of the sound stimulus. Moreover, if a pulse does
occur during
a particular cycle, its time of occurrence is restricted to that part
of the cycle
near the pressure peak. A study of the intervals between pulses would
reveal that
although the sequence of long and short intervals is unpredictable,
the probable
numbers of short ones and long ones that will occur in the future can
be estimated
from the corresponding numbers in these data.
It might be wondered at this stage just how the brain could make sense of such
a signal. If the pulse patterns of neuron 1 were the only information that the
brain received about the sound signal, its interpretation would
indeed be difficult.
Remember, however, that thousands of these axons exist. Many of them are known
to produce pulse patterns that, although not identical to the pattern of neuron
1, have nearly the same general description. Thus, the second pulse
pattern shown
in Fig. 5 shares all of the characteristics given for the pattern of
pulses generated
by neuron 1, but the two patterns are not identical. Imagine summing 100 such
pulse patterns. On any one particular pressure peak, some neurons,
say 20 on the
average, would generate a pulse; few neurons would generate a pulse in any of
the pressure valleys. Thus, every time about 20 pulses occurred within a short
time period, a pressure peak would most probably have occurred. If only one or
two pulses occurred in that period, a pressure valley most likely occurred then. Simply observing how the total number of
pulses occurring on those 100 axons varied with time would give the observer a
rather accurate indication of the pressure waveform.
The uncertainty concerning just when a neuron is going to generate
the next pulse
is not confined to the neurons leading from the ear. All neurons which respond
to tones, even those located in the brain's farthest reaches15, do so
by generating
pulse patterns which contain a considerable measure of uncertainty.
It seems clear
from this neuronal variability that the same tone will be represented
differently
in the brain at different times. Data showing unpredictable aspects could also
be presented for the responses produced by other types of sensory
stimuli. Neurons
in the brain responding to flashes of light generate different pulse trains in
response to identical repetitions of the same flash.16 Moreover,
uncertainties
are not confined to the sensory systems of the brain. Neurons such as
that depicted
in Fig. 1, which send pulses out of the spinal cord to control the muscles of
the body also display variability in their discharge patterns, although those
pulse patterns are much more nearly predictable than the ones shown in Fig. 5.
In general, it appears that all neurons have some degree of
unpredictability connected
with the generation of their axon pulses.
The Neuron as a Computing Element
Some of the information-transferring functions of the single neuron
are now fairly
well understood, at least in general principles. Although the details
of the generation
of the nerve pulses and the release of chemical transmitters by the different
kinds of neurons are not yet known, there is no longer any real
disagreement about
the reality of these basic neuronal processes themselves. One neuron does not,
however, make a brain. It takes many of them working together to make
up the simplest
kind of brain. It is this very area of interconnections and
interactions between
neurons that brain scientists are now just beginning to investigate.9
Unfortunately,
the research is so new and the complexides of the brain so great that not much
can be said yet of a positive nature. Although a considerable amount is known
about where the axons of individual neurons begin and end and about which parts
of the brain are related to which functions, the particular
interactions between
neurons by which the brain processes and stores the vast amount of information
it receives are almost wholly unknown. In view of this ignorance, it is clearly
impossible to state whether or not the brain is constructed like a
digital computer,
or anything else for that matter. However, because of the similarities in their
overall information processing abilities, the brain and the digital
computer have
been considered by some to he the same type of mechanism.2 To appreciate just
how computer processes might he considered models of brain processes, it will
he useful at this point to compare certain aspects of the two systems.
The basic budding blocks of the digital computer are called logic
elements. Each
of these elements is an electronic device that has a single output
terminal which
can have only one of two possible voltage levels on it. If the sum of the input
voltage is more positive than a certain threshold level, say for
convenience 10
The wonder is that the brain is able to achieve such highly reliable results with basic building blocks whose characteristics are describable only in probabilistic terms.
mV, the output is set to one level, say 100 mV. When the input voltage is below
the threshold level, the output voltage assumes its other level, say
zero volts.
Thus, the output level of the device is either zero or 100 mV, and
the particular
level which exists at any one time depends on whether or not the input voltage
exceeds the threshold level, 10 mV. Aside from the fact that its output voltage
level does not automatically reset itself to zero upon reaching the
100 mV level,
a property which could be easily added, the behavior of the logic
element is strikingly
reminiscent of the neuron membrane (cf. Fig. 2). This resemblance is
no accident,
for the first logic element was in fact developed as a model for a
patch of neuron membrane.17 The communication of one element with another is also
similar to the
communication between neurons. Although no chemical intermediates are involved,
the output of one logic element is conveyed via wires to the inputs
of other elements,
where it causes voltage changes to appear.
There are differences, however, which do exist between the neuron and the logic
element. For one, the particular way in which these input voltage
levels are handled
by a modern digital computer's logic elements is different from the
way in which
neurons handle incoming pulses. To put it simply, a logic element
generates pulses
on its output lines in response to single input pulses, whereas the
neuron generally
needs many pulses before it can produce a pulse. But this difference should not
be considered an essential one, for it is possible to build computers
from a different
type of logic element, one which, like a neuron, requires the summation of many
input pulses before an output pulse is generated. In fact this summing type of
logic element is a more powerful computing element than the type operating by
means of single pulses."18
There is a second and apparently more fundamental difference that
exists between
the neuron and any type of logic element now in use. On the one hand, the logic
element is a completely deterministic device that, if working properly, always
generates an output pulse when the proper input pulse or pulses have
been received.
Any uncertainty in the timing or size of the pulses is considered a cause for
concern and steps are taken to make these uncertainties as small as possible.
On the other hand, uncertainty seems to he a basic property of a
neuron. Consider,
for example, the situation shown in Fig. 5. It is impossible to
predict, by any
known methods, whether neuron 1 will emit an output pulse during any particular
cycle of the sound wave. It cannot be argued that this uncertainty is simply a
matter of particularly unfavorable conditions. For no matter how loud or soft
the sound is made, the pattern of output pulses can still he described only in
probabilities and not certainties. Furthermore, as was already
pointed out, these
uncertainties are not con
Present scientific evidence does not prove or disprove the existence of the soul nor prove or disprove that humans are only biological machines.
fined to neurons handling sound information, but are characteristic,
to some extent,
of all neurons studied. It should therefore come as no surprise to learn that
most of the mathematical descriptions of neural pulse trains use
statistical and
probabilistic methods.19
As so little is known about detailed interneuronal functioning, it is
exceedingly
difficult to try to contrast the operation of the brain at any level
of organization
other than that of the basic building blocks. It would appear, however, based
on the differing characteristics of their basic building blocks, that the brain
and the computer will prove to be very different. The modern digital computer
is a completely deterministic machine, dependent for proper functioning on the
unfailing operation of every single electronic component. By
contrast, the brain
can apparently tolerate considerable variability in the response
characteristics
of all of its neurons. Moreover, experiments have failed to show that the brain
is dependent in its operations on any one neuron.20 The wonder is that the
brain is able to achieve such highly reliable results with basic
building blocks
whose characteristics are describable only in probabilistic terms.
IMPLICATIONS FOR CHRISTIANS
Lessons describing the characteristics of single neurons or chemical
transmitters
are not scheduled for inclusion in the educational curricula of the Christian
Church. Yet modern scientific study of the brain is of importance to Christians
because of the intimate relationship which exists between the brain
and the mind.
("Brain" is here used to mean the physical organ. "Mind" is
used in the psychological sense to mean "the totality of
conscious and unconscious
mental processes and activities21 of a person.) For it cannot be
denied that
altering and controlling the human brain has proven capable of
altering the most
intimate and personal aspects of human experience.7 The use of
antipsychotic drugs
to control schizophrenia is but one example of the many ways in which drugs can
be used to change fundamental aspects of human personality. There are
also electrical22
and surgical23 means of altering the brain which greatly change
human experience.
In short, in its rapid development of ways in which to exert direct
control over
the brain, the neuroscienees are also learning to control the human mind.
Ethical Implications
Many of the practical implications for Christians, and for other
ethically minded
people, of the new types of biomedical control have been clearly
stated by others
and do not need to he completely restated here.24 However, it would be well to
consider briefly one area of ethical concern which has particular relevance to
brain research. This area concerns questions on the use and abuse of power.
Perhaps the most important point to he raised regarding the exercise of the new powers that neuroscience is creating is the one
raised many years ago by C. S. Lewis.2,3 In eloquent language, he
pointed out that
the powers created by science are never wielded by humanity as a whole, but by
the small minority of people who happen to be in control at the time. It must
be granted that so far, in this country, the powers of brain control have been
used mostly for beneficial purposes, such as controlling
schizophrenia and relieving
the symptoms of Parkinson's disease. Indeed, it is with the long range hope of
learning how to prevent and alleviate diseases and malfunctions of
the brain that
the large amount of federal support for brain research is granted.
Yet, regardless
of original motives, new powers of control are being created, and once created,
these powers will be available to future controllers, Whoever they may he.
As an example of the possible mischief that could be accomplished, consider the
method recently suggested by two brain scientists for the control of
violent crimes.
Under this method, parolees, high risk ex-convicts, and people on bail would be
required to wear physiological monitoring equipment connected by tiny two-way
radios to a computer.25 The computer's programs would continually monitor
the signals telemetered to it by each radio. Whenever the programs detected an
excited physiological state in a subject located in a suspicious
place, an impending
crime would be diagnosed. Police would be dispatched to the scene or
an electrical
shock would be applied to certain of the subject's brain centers, causing him
to forget or abandon the project. Such a system is technically
feasible now, but
would be of little or no value, because only the crudest of estimates
of the state
of mind of a person can currently be constructed from physiological
data, including
brain signals. Even so, the subject, with constant surveillance of
both his external
and internal worlds and with the continual threat of instantaneous
outside intervention,
would stiffer a much more profound loss of privacy and free will than
in prison.
These potential losses will no doubt become greater as the years go
by, for brain
scientists will be able to make increasingly accurate judgments about
a subject's
mental state and be able to influence it more exactly. Some may argue
that though
regrettable, such effects would be tolerable, for they would be confined to a
small criminal segment of the population. However, history, including that of
some countries during this very year, teaches us that anyone, irrespective of
his actual offenses, can be declared a lawbreaker by the powerful.
Moreover, many
governments have demonstrated that the number of oppressed need not be a small
number nor only a small segment of the population. Science is forging
unique tools
of great power. To believe that these tools may not someday be used ruthlessly
against powerless people is to ignore the lessons of history. It is
also to ignore
the Biblical lesson that the kingdom of God is not yet established on
the earth,26
and that evil still exists.
There are several other ethical aspects of brain research that have
been raised24 that must be at least mentioned here. On the one hand, it is possible that some
people without outside coercion will voluntarily use the fruits of
brain research
for self-degradation and dehumanization. The contemporary drug
culture has graphically
pointed out just how far this process can go. On the other hand, much of the truly beneficial knowledge that is
being developed
will, for a long time to come, be readily available only to those
limited segments
of our country's and the world's populations that are able to obtain adequate
medical care. Like all inequities, this distribution presents serious ethical
problems. Taken together, all of these concerns indicate that further
brain studies
should he approached with great caution. It will require great wisdom to plan
future research so as to obtain the maximum of beneficial results and
at the same
time to develop adequate safeguards against the misuse of the resulting powers.
Christians, with the fear of the Lord which is the beginning of wisdom,27 have
important roles to play in this planning.
Theological Implications
The increasing ability of brain scientists to understand and
manipulate the human
brain and mind by using the techniques of the physical sciences has goaded some
people to the belief that the brain must operate totally according to the same
basic physical and chemical priciples that govern inanimate
objects.1,2 Implicit
in this belief is a conviction that the mind is some aspect of the
brain's functioning,
with no existence apart from the brain. For it is without question
that the brain
controls the muscles of speaking and acting (cf. Fig. 1). If the
brain's activities,
and hence the person's words and deeds, were to be totally
explainable by physical
and chemical principles, then neither the brain itself nor anything acting on
it would be exempt from following these principles. In short, the
mind would have
to be a manifestation of the activity of the brain, totally
explainable by scientific
principles, or it would have to be a totally passive spectator.
Although mechanistic interpretations of human behavior are not new,28 there now
seems to be fresh evidence to support these positions. What should we think of
such hypotheses? Is a belief that the brain and the mind are just
parts of a biological
machine compatible with Christian beliefs?
Most Christians through the centuries have answered the last question
with a resounding,
"No". They believed that the essence of every person is a
non-material
immortal soul. Orthodox Christians still believe that the soul goes immediately
to its reward upon the death of the body. There, they believe, the
soul will remain,
independent of a body, until it is joined to a new and different kind of body
at the final resurrection. 29
It should be clear by this point in the paper that scientific evidence is not
capable of deciding this basic disagreement between most Christians
and the mechanists.
The human brain's array of more than 1010 neurons, interconnected by means of
neuronal pulses and other mechanisms, is of a complexity far beyond the ability
of modern science to analyze and describe completely. The point was well summed
up by Dr. H. Davis, a distinguished senior neurophysinlogist. At a
recent meeting
of the Society for Neuroscience, he referred to the relationship
between the brain
and the mind as the neuroscienees' toughest unsolved problem.30 He
further states
that neuroscientists have learned not even to use physiological and
psychological
terms in the same sentence, because of the mysterious gap that exists between
them. Some scientists even go so far as to say that the scientific tools needed
to tackle that gap have not even been developed yet.20 In short, understanding of the
neural principles governing the brain's "higher" functions,
with which
the soul and the mind are associated, is much too rudimentary in nature either
to support or to attack the traditional Christian position at this
time. The first
conclusion to be reached, therefore, is that present scientific evidence does
not prove or disprove the existence of the soul nor prove or disprove
that humans
are only biological machines.
As brain research continues, however, knowledge of the workings of
the brain will
undoubtedly continue to grow. Increasingly complex models of neurons
and neuronal
interactions will be developed and some would see no barriers to
eventually achieving
arbitrarily detailed explanations of the brain. If that happened, there would
be no need to talk about the soul, for all human actions would be predictable
by scientific principles. It appears, however, that there may be
naturally imposed
limits to the ability of science to describe brain behavior. As we have seen,
the present study of neurons has indicated that the uncertainties
connected with
the times of occurrence of the neuronal pulses are beyond current deterministic
explanation. Thus, even if future scientific advances would make it possible to
construct mechanistic models of each neuron in a particular brain and of all of
their interconnections, it would seem unlikely that the actions of
the brain model
would be exactly those of the brain itself. For, as far as we can see now, the
model of each neuron and perhaps each neural conection would have to
include elements
of uncertainty. Some models of the neurons represent these
uncertainties by means
of random variations in the threshold voltage level.31 Others
include uncertainties
in the times at which the packets of chemical transmitters are discharged into
the synaptic cleft.32 By these and other means, most neuron models
now incorporate
an element of random behavior.19
Ever since the inception of quantum theory, scientists have suggested that the
uncertainties of position and movement assigned by the theory to
elementary particles
might be important in the functioning of the brain.33 As these uncertainties on
the atomic scale are very small, various schemes have been suggested
for amplifying
their size33 so as to produce effects at higher levels of
organization. It would
appear that the neuron is just such an amplification device. For, if the neuron
models are accurate, uncertainties in the threshold level or in the
times of transmitter
release are events caused by uncertainties in the motion of a
relatively few atoms
and molecules. The unpredictabilities of these few particles would
thus be reflected
in the uncertain timing of the neuronal pulse, an event which controls the flow
of many thousands of atoms and molecules. In any ease, uncertainties,
originally
thought to hold only for atomic and molecular events, appear to be a
fundamental
characteristic of pulse events occurring in the neuron. And as the
neuron is the
basic building block of the brain, uncertainty is thereby introduced into the
highest levels of its organization.
Science may never be able to decide whether or not human beings have free will or a soul.
A word of caution must he inserted here. The uncertainties observed in neural
behavior might possibly be removed by future studies. One possibility is that
investigations pushed to molecular levels of organization might yield
deterministic
descriptions of the emission of neurotransmitters by the vesicles.
This particular
eventuality seems very unlikely, for current investigations show that the times
of neurotransmitter packet release do indeed seem to have random distributions.34
Another possibility is that groups of neurons may be found which together have
deterministic behavior. A logic element is an example of such a
device. Its deterministic
electrical output is made up of many electrons and other charge carriers, each
of which can be described only in probabilistic terms. So far, however, there
is no evidence of any groupings of neurons which produce completely
deterministic
outputs. Thus it would appear that some degree of uncertainty is
indeed a fundamental
characteristic of the triggering of neuronal pulses.
The theological implications of uncertainties in the functioning of individual
neurons are unclear. Some have argued that unpredictable events in
the brain would
reduce the control that an individual has over his own thoughts and actions.35
Others feel that uncertainty connected with brain events would
provide a mechanism
for free will to act that would not disturb the predictability of
physical events.33
Under this scheme, the will would be able to alter the individual brain events
which happened at particular instants without changing the
statistical properties
of the events, which would be under the control of physical
principles. Whatever
the merits of these speculations, the existence of fundamental uncertainties in
the timing of neuronal events would mean, subject to the qualifications given
in the preceding paragraph, that science would never be able to
construct a completely
deterministic explanation of the functioning of the brain. The uncertainties in
this explanation could very well he large enough to produce
uncertainties in the
basic decision-making processes of the brain, processes commonly
associated with
the will and the soul. Thus, a second conclusion can be drawn;
science may never
he able to decide whether or not human beings have free will or, by
implication,
a soul. In any ease, the decision is a very long way off.
It would he irreverent to end this section and this paper without reporting the
feeling of awe and wonder that often steals over neuroscientists as
we contemplate
the workings of the brain. Everything is so complex, yet, when understood, all
of the parts prove to he beautifully fitted for the functions that
they fulfill.
Surely we are fearfully and wonderfully made.36
REFERENCES
1B. F. Skinner, Beyond Freedom and Dignity, Alfred A. Knopf, New York, 1971.
2D. E. Wonidridge, The Machinery of the Brain, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1963.
3C. S. Lewis, The Abolition of Moo, MacMillan, New York, 1947, Chapter 3.
4C. S. Lewis, That Hideous Strength, MacMillan, New York, 1947.
5B. Katz, Nerve, Muscle and Synapse, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1966.
6G, D. Pappas and D. P. Purpura, Structure and Function of
Synapses, Raven, New York, 1972, Chapter 1.
7J. Axelrod, "Ncnrotransmitters," Scientific American, 230,
58 (1974).
8J. C. Eccles, The Understanding of the Brain, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1973.
9C. H. Shepard, The Synaptic Organization of the Brain, Oxford University,
New York, 1974, p. 118 ff.
10F. 0. Schmitt, as reported in Fourth Annual Meeting; Society
for Neuroscience, BIS Conference Report 38, BRA Publications Office, University of California, Los Angeles, 1975, p. 13.
11R. Llinas, R. Baker and C. Sotelo. "Eleetrotonic coupling
between neutrons
in eat interior olive," J. Neurophysiology, 37, 560 (1974).
12L. L, Iversea, "Dopamine receptors in the brain," Science 188, 1084
(1975).
13 Seeman and T. Lee, "Antipsyclsotie drugs: direct correlation between
clinical potency and presynaptie action on dopamine neutrons,"
Science 188,
1217-1219 (20 June 1975).
14J. F. Rose, J. F. Brugge, D. J. Anderson, J. E. Hind,
"Pbaselocked response
to low-frequency tones in single auditory nerve fibers of the
squirrel monkey,"
J. Neurophysiology 30, 769 (1967).
15J. F. Brugge and M. M. Merzenieh, "Responses of neurons in
auditory cortex
of the macaque monkey to monaural and binaural stimulation," J,
Neurophysinlogy
36, 1138 (1973),
16M. J. Wright and H. Ikeda, "Processing of spatial and temporal
information
in the visual system," in F, 0. Schmitt and F. C. Worden (Eds.),
The Neurosciences:
Third Study Program, MIT Press, Cambridge, 1974.
170. H. Schmitt, "Au electrical theory of nerve impulse propagation,"
American J. Physiology 119, 399 (1937).
18B. 0. Winder, "Threshold logic will cut costs, especially with
boost from
LSI," Electronics 41, No. 11, 94 (1968).
19 E. Fienberg, "Stochastic models for single neuron firing trains: a
survey," Biometrics 30, 399 (1974).
20G. 5. Stent, "Limits to the scientific understanding of man,"
Science 187, 1052 (1975).
22 M. B. Delgado, Physical Control of the Mind; Toward a
Psychocivilized Society, Harper & Row, New York, 1969.
23V, H. Mark and F. B. Ervio, Violence and the Brain, Harper & Bow, New York
(1970).
24L. B. Kass, "The new biology: what price relieving man's estate,"
Science 174, 779 (1971).
25W. Tebbets, "Next step: electronic brain control of
parolees," Pacific
News Service, in Capital Times (Madison, Wis.) Thursday, February 8, 1973.
26M. Burrows, An Outline of Biblical Theology, Westminster,
Philadelphia, 1946, p. 188 ft.
27Proverbs 9:10.
28S. L. Jaki, The Relevance of Physics, University of Chicago,
Chicago, 1966, Chapter
9.
29Burrows, op. cit., p. 194 ff.
30H. Davis, as reported in Fourth Annual Meeting; Society for
Neuroscience, BIS Conference Report 38, BRI Publications Office, University of
California, Los Angeles, 1975,
p. 12.
31C. D. Geisler and J. M. Goldberg, "A stochastic model of the repetitive
activity of neurons," Biophysical J. 6, 54 (1966).
321R. B. Stein, "Some models of neuronal variability," Biophysical J.
7, 37-67 (1967).
33A. 0. Comes, "The brain-consciousness problem in contemporary brain research," in J. C. Eccles (Ed.), Brain
and Conscious Experience, Springer-Vedag, New York, 1965.
34B. Katz, op. cit., Chapter 9.
35D. M. MacKay, in discussion following Gomes' paper, op. cit., p. 463 ff.
36PsaIm 139:14.